Sarah Perry is a resident blogger visiting us from her home turf at The View from Hell.
A selective sweep occurs when a new, beneficial gene mutation appears and quickly sweeps across a population, erasing the genetic diversity that existed prior to the sweep. Similarly, languages have “swept” across continents as the cultures they belonged to gained unbeatable advantages (often agricultural or military), resulting in losses of language diversity from earliest human history to the present day. Today, half the population of the world speaks one of only thirteen languages.
These are not controversial claims. More controversial is the idea that human prehistory (and even history) hosted a wide variety of human consciousness, not just language, and that these disparate kinds of subjective consciousness were destroyed upon contact with new forms of consciousness. Most dramatically, Julian Jaynes famously argued (in The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind) that human consciousness changed drastically in the past few thousand years, from an archaic bicameral form in which one side of the brain shouted orders and the other obeyed, to a modern, introspective form. My claim is not so extreme: I simply argue that there are and have been many forms of human consciousness, varying in particular ways, that we retain the “hardware” capability for many forms of consciousness, and that humans are constrained into particular mental states by their cultures, especially through group ritual (or lack thereof). In order to explore this claim, it is helpful to think about our own form of consciousness in detail – a form of consciousness that is novel, contagious, and perhaps detrimental to human flourishing compared with more evolutionarily tested forms of consciousness running on the same hardware.
Scholastic-Industrial Consciousness
The form of consciousness that you and I share (if you are reading this) was likely not the only one to sweep across new territory, erasing previous variation. But it is the most recent such sweep, and it has been a dramatic one. It provides particular ways of subjectively experiencing time, identity, the self, other people, external reality, and the divine. (E. Richard Sorenson lists the consciousness variants as sense-of-name, sense-of-space, sense-of-number, sense-of-truth, and sense-of-emotion.) Ours is a literate kind of consciousness, gathering momentum with the advent of printing and achieving its ultimate realization (though with some subversion) in the form of the internet. Since it appears to be transmitted by schooling and since it is the form of consciousness most conducive to industrialization, it may be thought of as scholastic-industrial consciousness.
Axes of Variation of Consciousness
Here I will explore a few of the most dramatic variations in the determinants of human experience. They are difficult to notice because they form the unquestioned background of our experience; our present senses of time, identity, and the self seem to be immutable aspects of human experience, but historical and cross-cultural study reveals that they do vary significantly.
Time
Nick Szabo writes that the time-rate wage – selling one’s time as a measure of sacrifice, as opposed to serfdom or piece work – accelerated around the fourteenth century in Europe, as “mechanical clocks, bell towers, and sandglasses provided the world’s first fair and fungible” measure of time. Increasingly reliable, precise, and accessible measurement of time increased productivity and material well-being in Europe even prior to the printing press. But submitting to the new form of time was itself a sacrifice. Coordinated punctuality comes at the cost of each person living most of his hours aware of the wider world’s standard time, which is a novel way of being human. In fourteenth-century France and for many centuries after, when human labor was irreplaceably valuable and there were many gains available from increased coordination, the trade-off made sense. Now that human labor is becoming less valuable and more replaceable, forcing people to live bound by world standard time might be becoming harder to justify.
Identity
In the scholastic-industrial world, each person is assigned a legal name at birth, and carries that same name and identity until death. A modern government identity accumulates reams of information, now easily attached to it via electronic databases. From preschool to college, educational information becomes attached to one’s identity, and this process does not stop at employment; information about experience, taxes, consumption, credit, medical involvement, and more accumulates on one’s “permanent record.”
Maintenance of reputation is not novel; it is a key element of human nature, common to all societies. But not all societies even have fixed names; those outside the reach of complex governments experience more flexible identities. Sorenson says:
In these preconquest regions of New Guinea names were rarely binding. What one was called varied according to time, place, mood, and setting. Names were improvised, not formally bestowed, and naming (much like local language flexibility) was often a kind of humorous exploratory play. New names could be quickly coined, often whimsically from events and situations, with a new one coming up at any time….One girl was called “Aidpost” following her excitement about the first one in the region; another was called “Sleepgood” by a new friend who liked sleeping with her.
Daniel Everett reports a similar laxity toward identity among the Pirahã (hunter-gatherers in the Amazon), and notes that the Pirahã often trade names with spirits that they meet in the jungle (at p. 9).
Certainly, the volume of information attached to a modern identity is novel. The internet at first seemed to provide escape from invasive surveillance through anonymous or at least pseudonymic identities; unfortunately, privacy is inimical to the interests of government and consumer product distributors, and is rapidly being eroded.
The Self
Mirrors only became common in the nineteenth century; before, they were luxury items owned only by the rich. Access to mirrors is a novelty, and likely a harmful one.
In Others In Mind: Social Origins of Self-Consciousness, Philippe Rochat describes an essential and tragic feature of our experience as humans: an irreconcilable gap between the beloved, special self as experienced in the first person, and the neutrally-evaluated self as experienced in the third person, imagined through the eyes of others. One’s first-person self image tends to be inflated and idealized, whereas the third-person self image tends to be deflated; reminders of this distance are demoralizing.
When people without access to mirrors (or clear water in which to view their reflections) are first exposed to them, their reaction tends to be very negative. Rochat quotes the anthropologist Edmund Carpenter’s description of showing mirrors to the Biamis of Papua New Guinea for the first time, a phenomenon Carpenter calls “the tribal terror of self-recognition”:
After a first frightening reaction, they became paralyzed, covering their mouths and hiding their heads – they stood transfixed looking at their own images, only their stomach muscles betraying great tension.
Why is their reaction negative, and not positive? It is that the first-person perspective of the self tends to be idealized compared to accurate, objective information; the more of this kind of information that becomes available (or unavoidable), the more each person will feel the shame and embarrassment from awareness of the irreconcilable gap between his first-person specialness and his third-person averageness.
There are many “mirrors”—novel sources of accurate information about the self—in our twenty-first century world. School is one such mirror; grades and test scores measure one’s intelligence and capacity for self-inhibition, but just as importantly, peers determine one’s “erotic ranking” in the social hierarchy, as the sociologist Randall Collins terms it. Of the school sexual scene “mirror,” he says:
…although the proportion of the population whose sex lives are highly active is small, this prestige hierarchy nevertheless has an effect on persons ranked throughout. Particularly among young persons living in public sexual negotiation scenes, there is a high level of attention paid to erotic stratification criteria, and acute awareness of who occupies what rank in the community’s ratings….
The popular crowd is the sexual elite. Being in the center of attention gives greater solidarity, closer identification with the symbols of the group, and greater self-confidence. Conversely, those on the outskirts of the group, or who are excluded from it, manifest just the opposite qualities. Being part of the sociable/erotic elite produces an attitude of arrogance; the elite know who they are, and the enclosed, high-information structure of the scene makes visible the ranking of those lower down as well.
(Interaction Ritual Chains, p. 253; citations omitted; emphasis mine.)
There are many more “mirrors” available to us today; photography in all its forms is a mirror, and internet social networks are mirrors. Our modern selves are very exposed to third-person, deflating information about the idealized self. At the same time, say Rochat, “Rich contemporary cultures promote individual development, the individual expression and management of self-presentation. They foster self-idealization.”
Roy Baumeister (Meanings of Life, chapters five and six) traces the modern obsession with self to the erosion of other sources of value and meaning in life, such as religion, shared morality, and tradition:
When people say they need to find themselves, often what they really mean is that they want a meaningful life….People have always had selves, but selves have not always had to carry the burden of supplying meaning to life in such a far-reaching fashion. The reason for the modern fascination with the self, then, is that the self has been made into a fundamental and powerful source of value in modern life.
(Baumeister, pp. 77-78)
Since sources of meaning and value are now so rare, the self has had to take on the novel burden of providing value and fulfillment (an idealized future state that justifies unpleasantness in the present), as in the novel modern idea of the “career”:
The assumption is that your work will elevate you to a position of eminence that will elicit respect, admiration, and acclaim from others, as well as allowing you to feel self-respect and self-esteem. Many people hold some mythical view of career success that promises personal fulfillment. They imagine that reaching certain goals will be automatically accompanied by living happily ever after. (Baumeister, p. 125)
The novel function of the self as the primary locus of meaning, especially in an environment offering increasing amounts of accurate (which is to say painful) information about the self, is hard on modern humans. One popular option is self-deception; normal, healthy people tend to have an unrealistically positive self image, to exaggerate or overestimate the control they have over their lives, and to be unreasonably optimistic. “Illusions, distortions, and self-deception appear to be integral to the way normal, well-adjusted people perceive the world,” says Baumeister (p. 224). “Seeing things as they really are is associated with depression and low self-esteem.” Where self-deception fails, there are other routes to escape the painful self. Elsewhere, Baumeister argues that behaviors such as alcoholism, binge eating, sexual masochism, charismatic religion, spirituality, and even suicide function as escapes from the overburdened, embarrassed, shameful modern self.
In the rest of this essay I will propose a rudimentary sketch of a more salubrious escape plan from the novel modern self and the hardships that scholastic-industrial consciousness imposes upon us.
Constrained Toward Consciousness
Human consciousness is not one singular experience shared by all; there are varieties of human consciousness. Entering altered states of consciousness demonstrates that there are many possible states, and the ordinary, everyday consciousness of different people also varies.
Many animals experience consciousness in the form of awareness of the outside world and of self. The form of consciousness unique to and universal among humans, as explained in Philippe Rochat’s Others In Mind mentioned above), is the awareness of oneself from the imagined perspective of other minds. During development, we are constrained toward this form of consciousness by the words and behavior of others: we are forced, by others, to be aware of ourselves from their perspective—to have others in mind. Others influence our mental states down to the existence of human consciousness itself. We may expect that groups have great power to affect our mental states. Indeed, group presence, group awareness, and group expectations are necessary ingredients for the induction of certain ritual mental states.
As scholastic-industrial consciousness has made our selves more weighty and painful, increasing the need for the loss of self through ritual, the opportunity for such rituals has decreased. As we have become more literate and rational, a process begun during the Enlightenment and accelerating throughout the twentieth century, group rituals and the mental states that they induce have been lost. Those rituals that are left tend to be of the spectacle variety rather than participatory – listening to music rather than making it, watching rather than dancing. It is a great loss. Rituals function, on the one hand, as “social vitamins” – necessary ingredients for human flourishing that are provided by ancestral social “diets” but frequently left out of modern lives. On the other hand, rituals are necessary social glue, connecting small communities and helping their members to cooperate and feel a sense of social belonging.
Hiving and Rhythmic Entrainment
One proponent of the theory that group ritual is a necessary ingredient for achieving desirable states of consciousness is the psychologist Jonathan Haidt. Haidt refers to this as “the hive hypothesis,” explored in his essential book The Righteous Mind as well as a recent paper. (Also see this excellent exploration of hiving through the loss of self in religion and politics by fellow Ribbonfarm guest blogger Kevin Simler.) The hive hypothesis states that “people need to lose themselves occasionally by becoming part of an emergent social organism in order to reach the highest levels of human flourishing.” The joy of synchronized motion, and the loss of self in something greater, he argues, lead to stronger social cohesion and greater personal well-being for participants.
Humans are some of the only animals capable of rhythmic entrainment, synchronized motion to a musical beat. It has a special place in human culture, from dancing to military drilling, from children’s rhythm games to North Korea’s Mass Games. Synchronized, rhythmic motion eases the pain of strenuous effort, facilitates cooperation, makes people braver in the face of formidable opposition, and (on a darker note) makes them more willing to engage in aggression against a perceived out-group. Rhythmic entrainment is a powerful hack that allows the painful self (and self-interest) to be put aside in favor of the joy of solidarity. And, to our detriment, we have ever fewer opportunities to engage in rhythmic rituals today.
Donald Brown’s celebrated list of human universals, a list of characteristics proposed to be common to all human groups ever studied, includes many entries on music, including “music related in part to dance” and “music related in part to religion.” The Pirahã use several kinds of language, including regular speech, a whistling language, and a musical, sung language. The musical language, importantly, is used for dancing and contacting spirits. The Pirahã, Everett says, often dance for three days at a time without stopping. They achieve a different consciousness by performing rituals calibrated to evoke mental states that must remain opaque to those not affected.
There is something important in rhythmic rituals that cannot be fully captured in scholastic-industrial rational thought; it must be experienced to be understood. Nonetheless, it is not magical in the sense of being supernatural or outside the bounds of scholastic-industrial-style inquiry.
Authenticity, Sincerity, and the Suspicion of Ritual
When we identify something as a ritual, it is often to say that it is just a ritual, an empty behavior with no meaning. The performance of a ritual tells us nothing about the sincere feelings of the person performing it; rituals strike us as inauthentic. Shaking hands or saying “bless you” when someone sneezes are performed automatically, and in that way inauthentically. Randall Collins, cited above (in Interaction Ritual Chains), notes that hugging as a greeting did not emerge in the United States until the 1970s; this “more sincere” ritual has started edging out handshakes as the demonstrative greeting of choice. I have noticed the emergence of a new and even more conspicuously sincere ritual, the 20-second hug—which can be a very pleasant ritual, but imposes a great deal of intimacy (sincerity) within the space of a greeting.
Adam Seligman and Robert Weller, in their books Ritual and Its Consequences: An Essay on the Limits of Sincerity (2008) and Rethinking Pluralism: Ritual, Experience, and Ambiguity (2012), propose that the elevation of the sincere and authentic, and the distrust of ritual, is an important feature of modern consciousness. They trace the focus on personal intent (as opposed to ritual performance) to Immanuel Kant, for example. To be an atomized individual self, whose actions are under one’s rational control and express one’s sincere, authentic intent at all times is a peculiar, modern way to be human. Ritual is the normal way to be human. Our disdain for “empty rituals” is our loss: rituals are what help us navigate the necessary ambiguities of social life, into which rational codification can never fully penetrate.
So imagine “ritual,” but with a positive connotation. Rather than dismissing these behaviors as meaningless, look instead at how they elaborate meaning. And, perhaps more importantly, enjoy them without the sneaking suspicion that you’re not being authentic enough when you perform them.
Distracting the Watcher at the Gates of the Mind
Keith Johnstone describes a process of mental censorship that arises from an excessive cultural focus on the individual self as the locus of meaning. I apologize for the length of the quotation, but I promise it is necessary:
Suppose an eight-year-old writes a story about being chased down a mouse-hole by a monstrous spider. It’ll be perceived as ‘childish’ and no one will worry. If he writes the same story when he’s fourteen it may be taken as a sign of mental abnormality. Creating a story, or painting a picture, or making up a poem lay an adolescent wide open to criticism. He therefore has to fake everything so that he appears ‘sensitive’ or ‘witty’ or ‘tough’ or ‘intelligent’ according to the image he’s trying to establish in the eyes of other people. If he believed he was a transmitter, rather than a creator, then we’d be able to see what his talents really were.
We have an idea that art is self-expression—which historically is weird. An artist used to be seen as a medium through which something else operated. He was a servant of the God. Maybe a mask-maker would have fasted and prayed for a week before he had a vision of the Mask he was to carve, because no one wanted to se his Mask, they wanted to see the God’s. When Eskimos believed that each piece of bone only had one shape inside it, then the artist didn’t have to ‘think up’ an idea. He had to wait until he knew what was in there — and this is crucial. When he’d finished carving his friends couldn’t say ‘I’m a bit worried about that Nanook at the third igloo’, but only, ‘He made a mess getting that out!’ or ‘There are some very odd bits of bone about these days.’ These days of course the Eskimos get booklets giving illustrations of what will sell, but before we infected them, they were in contact with a source of inspiration that we are not. It’s no wonder that our artists are aberrant characters. It’s not surprising that great African sculptors end up carving coffee tables, or that the talent of our children dies the moment we expect them to become adult. Once we believe that art is self-expression, then the individual can be criticised not only for his skill or lack of skill, but simply for being what he is.
Schiller wrote of a ‘watcher at the gates of the mind’, who examines ideas too closely. He said that in the case of the creative mind ‘the intellect has withdrawn its watcher from the gates, and the ideas rush in pell-mell, and only then does it review and inspect the multitude.’ He said that uncreative people ‘are ashamed of the momentary passing madness which is found in all real creators . . . regarded in isolation, an idea may be quite insignificant, and venturesome in the extreme, but it may acquire importance from an idea that follows it; perhaps in collation with other ideas which seem equally absurd, it may be capable of furnishing a very serviceable link.’
Keith Johnstone, Impro, p. 78-79.
What is this “watcher at the gates of the mind” that crushes spontaneity, originality, and fun? A small study recently concluded that an explanation will be perceived as more satisfying if it has a neuroscience angle, even if the neuroscience angle is completely non-probative of its claims. So I am happy to say that I have a neuroscience explanation to offer: the censorious “watcher” likely lives in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
An even smaller study, for example, studied the brains of rappers, both reciting memorized verses and “freestyling” – inventing new lyrics on the fly. Under fMRI, subjects freestyling showed decreased activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Another study, measuring jazz musicians either playing previously memorized music and improvising new music, demonstrated the same pattern of activation: decreased DLPFC activity in improvising musicians.
So we have a candidate for the watcher at the gates of the mind. Arne Dietrich named this the “transient hypofrontality hypothesis”, proposing that what altered states such as “dreaming, endurance running, meditation, daydreaming, hypnosis, and various drug-induced states” have in common is a pattern of inhibition in the prefrontal cortex. Group rituals, especially rhythmic rituals (like endurance running), have the power to inhibit ordinary self-conscious social rumination and provide pleasurable ego-loss as well as social connection and bonding.
Finally, if that is not convincing enough, another line of research has suggested that any picture whatsoever – even one that is not probative of any fact – increases people’s confidence in facts when they are presented alongside them. I therefore present the most convincing argument in the world:
The Institutional Power of Rhythmic Ritual: Evidence from Self-Flagellation
In sixteenth century France, a new form of Catholic worship was rising in popularity. Penitential confraternities, brotherhoods of Catholic laymen, began to spread throughout France; by the end of the seventeenth century, every tiny village had its own chapter. But by the mid-eighteenth century, the penitential societies were beginning to lose social cohesion and break down. What made these societies so popular, and why did they decay after 1750?
At first, the penitential societies were based around a very strong and powerful ritual: that of self-flagellation. In the early days, members performed the rite of self-flagellation every week, and three times per week during Lent. By the early eighteenth century, however, the focus of the confraternities had shifted to emphasize meditation and community service; they dropped the rite of self-flagellation in favor of contemplation and charitable acts. Andrew Barnes explains:
Research on altered states of consciousness suggests that the performance of rituals generates a different psychological experience from that of practice of meditation. Ritual performance generates experiences of communitas. It rewards corporate religious activity in ways that meditation does not. It can be argued that the rituals performed by confraternities early in their existence were crucial for the maintenance of group and organizational cohesion.
Andrew Barnes, “From Ritual to Meditative Piety: Devotional Change in French Penitential Confraternities from the 16th to the 18th Century.” Journal of Ritual Studies 1:2:1-26, Summer 1987.
Toward the end, around 1731, the brothers perceived their problem as being one of internal discord and strife; their solutions were more regulation and more intricate statutes, rather than a return to rhythmic ritual. “The one trend in confraternal statutes more eye-catching than the decrease in ritual devotions is the increase in procedures to regulate and reduce internal discord,” says Barnes. “Internal discord was not a main concern in the statutes of earlier confraternities.”
Self-flagellation, in our scholastic-industrial society, is mostly mentioned as a punch line, if at all. That, I think, is scholastic-industrial consciousness undermining the power of other modes of consciousness that it can’t readily explain or understand. It is the kind of group rhythmic ritual that has the power to help us lose ourselves in something greater, and to bind us to each other more deeply – if that is what we want.
Some Words of Caution
I have analogized ritual to vitamins: we have need of them, our ancestral cultures provided them for us, and we suffer a kind of malnutrition without them. But even vitamins are dangerous in the wrong doses and under the wrong conditions. Ritual is very powerful, and should be regarded with the same respect as powerful psychoactive drugs.
In our hypermobile age, in which few people live among committed, long-term community members, we experience a chicken-egg problem: ritual is required to support community, but a community is required to support ritual (see diagram, elaborated from an analogy suggested by my husband). The subjective experience of ritual, divorced from a real community, may be pleasurable but ultimately unfulfilling.
Another danger is that the old rituals may not survive contact with us hungry ritual zombies. The Oaxacan shamaness María Sabina shared her hallucinogenic mushroom ritual with the ethnomycologist R. Gordon Wasson, and shortly after (during the 1960s and 70s) her village was overrun with tripping hippies running through the village naked with live turkeys dangling from their mouths. In María Sabina’s words, the mushrooms began to lose their power after the coming of the foreigners. Rituals are complex things, and their efficacy depends not only on chemical disinhibition, but on the beliefs and practices of the community within which they are performed.
Finally, ritual mental states sneak up on you; mere demonstration is often enough to transmit them. The anthropologist Felicitas Goodman, in her book Speaking in Tongues: A Cross-Cultural Study of Glossolalia, describes being overtaken by an intense ritual mental state herself:
Not even the ethnographer is immune. In one of my first conversations with the minister in Utzpak, he said that of course he would like me to receive the blessing and the promise of the Holy Spirit while I was in Utzpak; he would pray for me when I was ready. “However,” he continued, “the Spirit may come to you without either one of us actually asking for it.” And he was right.
…The last conscious memory I have of the episode to follow is that of thinking, “At home when I was a child, we were taught a little prayer to say before we sat down in church.” Then someplace in the church, I do not remember where, I leaned against something, I do not know what. I saw light, but then I was surrounded by light, or perhaps not, because the light was in me, and I was the light. In this light I saw words in black outline—or were they just letters?—descending upside down as if on a waterfall of light…. Never before had I felt this kind of luminous, ethereal, delightful happiness.
…I think I was obeying a cultural expectation: I had seen demonstrated many times how people went into dissociation. I was in the proper place at the proper time, thinking of prayer, and then it happened to me quite spontaneously and without any conscious effort on my part. It did not happen again because I intentionally blocked subsequent occurrences. I needed at all times to be in complete control of my faculties.
Most of the time, in most cultures, not just our own, we do need to be in complete control of our faculties. But we also need, sometimes, to dissolve into our groups, which presupposes the existence of such a group. While I think we are socially and emotionally starving without participatory group rituals, especially rhythmic rituals, I also think we must be very cautious in adding new or old rituals to our diet. The rebirth of ritual is the most deadly serious play.
Especially Recommended Readings
Johnstone, Keith. Impro: Improvisation and the Theatre. New York: Routledge, 1987.
Kavanagh, Aidan. On Liturgical Theology. New York: Pueblo Publishing Company, 1984.
Rochat, Philippe. Others In Mind: On the Social Origins of Self-Consciousness. Cambridge University Press, 2009.
Seligman, Adam, and Robert Weller. Rethinking Pluralism: Ritual, Experience, and Ambiguity. Oxford University Press, 2012.
This is some extremely interesting reading about the role of ritual and social structure in shaping human consciousness, perhaps even in the sense of maintaining a well-functioning one. It almost reminds me of Mircea Eliade’s “The Sacred and the Profane”, which studies some of the same things but from a different perspective as Eliade’s focus was more on how the structures of religious cosmology are integral to how humans form functional worldviews. (specifically the parts about how rituals are essential in the maintenance of the cosmologies in question)
Regarding rhythmic entrainment: The rightfully renowned historian William P. McNeill has written a fascinating book focusing on precisely this.
I recommend “Keeping Together in Time: Dance and Drill in Human History” to anyone interested in this topic, full of documented examples from many places and times. And fun to read.
I get the sense that there is another side to this whole thesis: that there are _new_ modes of consciousness being born all the time, but we just don’t recognize them yet. It only looks like a monoculture because we haven’t developed the right kind of color vision yet, so to speak, to detect differences that are already having consequences.
Your argument goes as follows: there used to be more consciousness-diversity. Much of the variety was to be found embodied in the variety of rhythmic social rituals that suppresses certain ego-anchoring brain functions. We are starved of enough such ritual outlets in modernity to experience the diversity. Therefore the current condition is a monoculture.
I think the problem with this argument is that you assume the rhythmic social ritual is the only space of conditions that draw out diversity in consciousness types.
This is a common view and leads to viewing individualism itself as a sort of impoverishing disease.
What if there are *other* kinds of varied conditions that can catalyze equal or greater diversity in consciousness software running on the same hardware? Even assuming you’re right that there is a growing poverty on the rhythmic-social-ritual front (I am not convinced yet that raves and things aren’t more than enough to replace tribal dances), that does not mean there aren’t other vectors of mind that aren’t getting enriched.
To make it specific, what if all our varied individualist, technology-absorbed behaviors of today are simply shades of consciousness variety we cannot yet tell apart? What if Twitter-mind and blog-commenting mind and VR mind are as distinct as shamanic-dance mind and BDSM-mind and praying-at-Church mind?
Actually, I think they are. We’re at the beginning of a new Cambrian explosion of consciousness-variety. In complex systems, when homogeneity appears at one level, diversity often explodes at the next level. So when the soup of competing replicator molecules got swept by RNA in the primordian soup, we gave up all those soupy little pre-DNA primordial creatures, but gained the DNA based diverse ecosystem.
This may not even be a subjective thing. It may be possible to quantify that relative complexity of the before and after in an absolute sense, in information theoretic terms.
“What if Twitter-mind and blog-commenting mind and VR mind are as distinct as shamanic-dance mind and BDSM-mind and praying-at-Church mind?”
Twitter and blog-commenting are very weak interactions. Writing as a communication tool is very one directional and very self involved. Especially w.r.t Twitter and Blog-commenting, you are mainly responding to your interpretation of an idea superimposed by your imagined archetype of the tweet/blog’s author. It’s all you. The presence of the tweet is nothing more than a small signal with very little dimensionality.
So there really is no “real-time” “back-and-forth” signals in Twitter and Blog-commenting.
I think “real-time” VR with “presence” may bring back the multi modal experience though.
Since colocation seems to be so important for inducing ritual mental states, it’s all about figuring out how to mimic colocation enough to induce these sensations – I’m very interested in experimenting with this. A friend was telling me tonight he just got to play with Oculus Rift – lots of possibilities there, though even telephone has likely not been explored enough this way.
New modes of consciousness: yes, I hope so! This made me think of the “new senses” that are starting to become possible – mapping information (of any kind) onto any pre-existing sensory modality, so people can see with their tongues http://www.radiolab.org/story/seeing-tongues/ or hear with their sense of touch http://www.radiolab.org/story/eagle-eyes/.
There are all kinds of streams of information that I would like to perceive in a different way that reading. Reading itself is a new way of being conscious, much as I tend to denitrage it here!
I was reminded reading this of a point Larry Harvey, Burning Man founder, made in his recent Long Now talk. See Chapter 6 “How ritual emerges” here (and watch the whole talk, even if you’re not into Burning Man): http://library.fora.tv/2014/10/20/Larry_Harvey_Why_the_Man_Keeps_Burning
The point I wonder about is how this theory of varieties of consciousness waxing and waning relates to Jaynes, who you tantalizingly mention at the beginning but don’t engage further with. I’ve been fascinated with the implications of his being right for some time (as Venkat knows) in terms of the idea that consciousness is something very new, we are still only grappling with, and that our religious impulse stems from (extending on Jaynes here) something like a hankering to a return to a simpler time when halluncinated gods told us what to do. I’d be interested in hat would it mean for your ideas, Sarah, if Jaynes was right?
Realized the Fora talks are now pay for play. Here’s Harvey saying similar things about ritual: http://www.zpub.com/burn/larry2.html
Yes – what if the “pains” are *growing* pains?
If you want to get into another consciousness try Drawing on the Right Side of the Brain. It goes through some drawing exercises and the results are neat. You enter some kind of zone where your focus on what you see is all that matters. Time is irrelevant to you. Having to answer a question out loud breaks it. I used it while washing bugs off my car, a mundane task that I usually rush and do half-assed, every bug gone.
“This may not even be a subjective thing. It may be possible to quantify that relative complexity of the before and after in an absolute sense, in information theoretic terms.”
Holy shit I’ve been doing exactly this – not in terms of information theory but in my own theoretical language – and I’d love to send you an email about it. I’ve got tens of thousands of words on it and I need to share it with somebody.
Can you give any sort of brief synopsis? What sort of ideas are you using in place of information theory? Do you mean to say you’re putting measurements to conscious systems, or just to the other sorts of examples above?
Contrasting individualism with ritual might not reveal much. Ritual is maybe better conceived in terms of niche. A niche is constructed by individual behaviour but the structure of the niche affects the setting and behaviour of individual behaviour. Niche gets us at this idea of co-creation through boundaries that comes up later in this series. The social boundaries interacting with individual behaviour are what creates meaning. Without a social context and setting an individuals innovation on a prior social form has no meaning. If i sing the best punk song ever in a church choir it is poorly fit to that niche. In it’s proper setting, the setting in which it was co-created, my individual innovation on that form has meaning and power.
Heh, looked for “dorsolateral” in Christof Koch’s autobiography, and found “It is there, in the prefrontal cortex, and specially in its dorsolateral division than the higher intellectual functions-problem solving, reasoning, and decision making-are located. Damage to this region will leave a person’s sensory modalities and memory intact, but her capacity to make rational decisions will be compromised: She will engage-and perseverate-in highly inappropriate behavior”. So that would seem to somewhat support the DLPFC ideas (and it’s kind of funny to see what would be the corresponding bias from the text in support of the watcher and the “appropriate” behavior).
“Inappropriate” as in exhibiting one’s penis to young girls, and shitting on the sidewalks downtown during business hours.
It is easy to get caught up in a quasi-fascist ritual mania, where ritual is abused by the ruling classes or the wannabe ruling classes. Brainwashing, consciousness raising, melting one’s mind into the group, etc.
Rituals are fine as long as they are understood for what they are, and left behind when one leaves the place of ritual.
Yeah, don’t like those things either. Then, there are other things which would be more likely to happen with the hypothetical DLPFC watcher leaving the guard down, most people would find inappropriate (at least in the society the author of the book lives in), and I don’t mind at all.
Don’t know under which category do the things that the author had in mind when writing that fall, and have a lot of admiration and respect towards their work and intellectual attitude. However, I think it would be fairer when writing about that topic to mention something about the other edge of the sword – that is to say, the positive potential of more disinhibited behavior.
I suggest considering “magic”, as it is properly defined (see, for example, ‘Blood of the Earth’ by Greer):
The art and science of causing changes in consciousness in accordance with will.
The meditative traditions consider effecting changes in consciousness to be science…
I’m not clear on how contemporary peer sexual-attractiveness rating has been changed by mirrors and photography. You may be more aware now of *why* you’re unattractive, but has attractiveness-inequality increased? Even if it has, is that due to the mirror, or other social changes?
A practical challenge with the idea of re-introducing group ritual is handling the transition between times of ritual self-loss and the times when “we do need to be in complete control of our faculties.” It seems to that during the period of ritual there wasn’t much Self (Agency) to get back to.
While group ritual is particular effective, new technologies of individual self-manipulation might offer the more flexible model for switching states.
cf Robert Anton Wilson’s various experiments on himself.
http://boingboing.net/2014/09/25/robert-anton-wilsons-cosmic.html
*experienceable misspelled as experiencable (…if only we would use cedillas in English)
Bottom of second paragraph after first graphic.
Nicely done; examples of how our consciousness now may well be quite different from what it once was, each accompanied by a hint of why the change could be considered undesirable. It would certainly be nice to go further, in a few different directions.
First, the normative aspect… I do think I like all some of these changes. Of course it’s only natural that “I” would like them, since my “I” is constructed by them. But still, what we really need is upsides and downsides.
Secondly, there has got to be more to these changes than such obvious reactions. If mirrors or clocks cause big enough changes to qualify as different sorts of consciousness, shouldn’t there be side-effects? I suppose we are only just starting to understand the side-benefits of, for example, meditation. But still, there must be some actual structural changes to brain function.
Thirdly, of course it’d be nice to list a whole lot more of these things.
Expanding on these in reverse… Reading is a huge vector for the spread of SI consciousness. Its most obvious effects are on how we perceive language. Movies also seem like a huge one. Accurate representational drawing also seems like a huge shift, though more playful drawing may be as well. I guess I’m getting more modern and specific here but… cars surely change our sense of place, and the Internet or just plain computers change our interaction with knowledge and memory. Woah, I didn’t even think about how many things could be added to the list until I started listing.
A list by itself isn’t that interesting, though; I mean there are plenty of things talking about how kids these days have a different self-image due to the internet. The interesting question is how these consciousness-shapers spread, what other consciousness-shapes might be like, and what we should do about it.
Rewinding to point two then. I can at least say that reading is known to cause changes in the way people listen to spoken language (see “Illiterate Brain”, Castro-Caldas); nonsense words are repeated more accurately by literate people. Probably without those changes, we would have less companies with nonsense-word names….
Googling some more, though, I find the work of one Stanislas Dehaene showing that learning to read causes our ability to recognize faces to be more lateralized; the left hemisphere’s facial recognition is repurposed to recognize letters and words. That’s hardly a dramatic change in consciousness, but still the sort of thing I wanted to find…
If there are similar neurological effects caused by familiarity with mirrors and clocks, my google skills are not strong enough. Still, I would suggest more complex changes are there.
I guess all I have to say about my own point 1 – normativity – is that I think the regulation provided by the DLPFC is actually pretty useful, when combined with uninhibited creativity. I think modern society comes with plenty of social machinery helping people be creative despite self-consciousness. Being able to switch between “generate” and “select” is a useful ability.
I would like to ask anyone who sees this comment for links regarding rituals stopping working, or spirits disappearing, after interaction with Westerners. I think it’s a really fascinating phenomenon and want to collect examples.
As for hugging strangers, there is a sharing of energies between both parties’ chakras. It is a gamble to hug a complete stranger, and can disrupt one’s inner balance with outside energies that are not in resonance.
Fist-bumps for strangers, hugs only for intimate friends, is a guideline by which some people abide.
Well I’m late to the party and there are about a dozen things I’d like to say in response to this epic post, but… I’ll limit myself just to three:
1. “Scholastic-industrial consciousness” is my new favorite term. It so aptly captures what the thing is.
2. I like your diagnosis of our current monoculture: too much self. It rings true to me. The older I get, the more I see my friends (and, I must admit, myself) struggling to escape from the self, to turn the damn thing off for a few hours here and there.
3. What do you make of the argument (as, incidentally, I once made on this blog ;) that consciousness is contagious because it’s more useful the more widely it’s shared? In other words, that there are network effects to consciousness, similar to the network effects from language and culture. To take that argument further: yes, we can lament the loss of diversity imposed by the monoculture, and we can resent the monoculture for causing so many problems (e.g. unhappiness), but the forces that gave rise to it are inexorable (economically speaking). It’s like the difference between living in the city and living in a small town. Yes, cities create their share of problems that towns don’t have, but on net cities solve more problems than they cause — and thus will continue to grow and grow and grow. And anyone who can’t adapt to city life is (all else equal, and in the long run) destined to be left behind.
Are you familiar with Robin Hanson’s predictions for what a future full of human ems will look like? He comes at this from a completely different angle, but he suggests that the humans who will be emulated and copied the most are the ones with the strongest “scholastic-industrial” minds. Why? Simply because they’re the most productive in a scholastic-industrial world. All the other types of minds and states of consciousness, which we’re eager to celebrate (and I really do count myself in that “we”), simply get outcompeted.
Yes/no/maybe?